Updated: 12/1/2005; 8:13:53 AM.
Bruce Landon's Weblog for Students
My Home Page Psych100 Psych200 Psych360 Psych330 EduTools News Landonline
        

Tuesday, November 22, 2005

Mildly depressed people more perceptive than others.

Surprisingly, people with mild depression are actually more tuned into the feelings of others than those who aren’t depressed, a team of Queen’s psychologists has discovered. “This was quite unexpected because we tend to think that the opposite is true,� says lead researcher Kate Harkness. “For example, people with depression are more likely to have problems in a number of social areas.�

[Science Blog -]
6:57:34 PM    comment

ELGG vs. Moodle - defusing a false dichotomy.

http://www.unisa.edu.au/odlaaconference/PPDF2s/13%20odlaa%20-%20Anderson.pdf

I've had a few people come up to me at conferences recently and ask me to compare ELGG and Moodle, and choose between them as if they were somehow mutually exclusive. Indeed, even within the Moodle community itself there seems to be a bit of dismissiveness about what ELGG does, and the notion that with just a couple of twists of code Moddle can easily replicate its functionality.

Well maybe, but this is what excited me so much about the paper linked to above by Terry Anderson and the work he describes taking place at Athabasca University. I had the pleasure of seeing Terry present on this recently and wish I could link to those powerpoints as I think the illustrate the point I'm trying to make better than the article does, but what is exciting for me is that Terry and Athabasca are putting together a large, production environment in which Moodle and ELGG will seemingly co-exist quite nicely, thank you very much, and take care of different problems. Hopefully I am not going to mangle this too much, but as I understood it, Moodle was being positioned to handle conventional 'course management' problems like the delivery of content, assessments, discussions. In Athabasca's case (and I'd argue in all of our cases, but that's another post) they also have to deal with a continuous uptake model, where instead of cohort-based programs they also have very much self-paced programs with differing start times. Thus they are using ELGG as one of the ways to build community "between" the space of courses, community that is formed not because of one's membership in a pre-ordained group or cohort but out of your interests. Sounds to me like a job for social software!

Can Moodle support similar ad-hoc community formation across course (and even institutional) boundaries? Maybe, and it sounds like we will find out fairly soon through upcoming releases. And bully for them if they can. But what I love about ELGG is that it is built from the groud up around the user and their connections as they key focus, rather than on 'courses' or 'content' (I'm not trying to levy a criticism at Moodle here as I like it very much as well). Far from being only a 'blogging' tool or a 'eportfolio' tool, what excites me about ELGG is that it is becoming a social networking 'framework' (o.k. you can dispute that term as much as you like) that while it has initially focused on tools to create blog posts and share files, isn't interested in restricting you to only its blogging tool (and why would it? RSS anyone?) and is looking at a whole set of other interesting apps (Calendaring? Synchronous tools?) that are also of intrinsic value but become even more useful if people can use them with other semantically related users.

Should elearning providers be looking to one single tool to provide all of these aspects and more? Maybe. Right now though, the best bet seems like trying to get the best solution possible through a set of provisional measures. Personally, I'm more interested in making these and others co-exist, and seeing if we can get the integration between them to be more than lame-ass 'pointing to their URLS' or simple single sign-on; if instead we see if we can get shared identity happening across a number of these services in a way that takes identity mean more than your username and password. - SWL

[EdTechPost]
6:47:42 PM    comment

Can distraction keep us from getting distracted?.

Attentional blink researchers Christian Olivers and Sander Nieuwenhuis noticed something curious when they conducted their experiments. When they were testing their stimuli, they felt that they could do the task better when they were distracted. Their participants also reported that they tended to perform better when they were a little unfocused on the task.

Attentional blink, as we’ve reported before, is a short period when we’re less able to notice an item flashed briefly in our visual field. The classic experiment involves a rapidly flashed sequence of random digits or letters — Olivers and Nieuwenhuis use letters flashed every 120 milliseconds. Then after a short interval, a digit is substituted for a letter. Nearly all observers notice this, even though they only see the letter for about a tenth of a second. However, if a second digit is flashed within the next half second, the rate of detection for this digit falls measurably.

So why would participants feel they were better at detecting the second digit when they weren’t as focused on the task? Was their introspection real, or did it just seem that way while they were performing the aggravating task of picking out digits from a rapid sequence of letters?

Olivers and Nieuwenhuis developed an experiment to systematically vary how focused participants were on the task. To decrease focus, they tried two methods — music and free association. Participants did the standard attentional blink task while listening to music (half of these people were told to listen for a “yell” which was inserted into the recording about 15 percent of the time, the other half simply listened to the music — it turned out the results were the same in both cases). Another group was told to think about the upcoming Christmas shopping season while they performed the task in silence. Finally, to try to increase focus, another group was offered a small reward (worth about a penny) each time they responded correctly. The computer gave these participants a running total of their accumulated wealth as the experiment progressed. Here are the results:

The orange line represents typical data for participants doing the task under normal conditions. Curiously, if the second digit is displayed immediately following the first digit, participants are very accurate. But if even a single letter is displayed inbetween the first digit and the second digit — corresponding to a period of 200 to 500 milliseconds — accuracy in identifying the second digit is substantially lower, declining from over 90 percent to the low 60-percent range.

The yellow line shows the results of the group that was rewarded for performance: their results were not significantly different from the standard testing condition. However, the other two groups (light blue, distracted with the shopping task, dark blue, distracted with music) showed substantially less attentional blink. In fact, the group listening to music had practically no blink at all.

Olivers and Nieuwenhuis offer three possible explanations for this phenomenon: the music and thinking about the holiday could have increased participants arousal. Their mood might also have been changed by the distracting tasks. Or, perhaps most interestingly, these distractions could have actually broadened the participants’ attention state, leading to an improved ability to detect the digits in the stream of letters.

Olivers, C.N.L., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2005). The beneficial effect of concurrent task-irrelevant mental activity on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 16(4), 265-269.

[Cognitive Daily]
5:54:02 PM    comment

Do new objects capture our attention?.

The picture below will link you to a quick animation. The blue ring will gradually get smaller until it obscures the three “8″s, then continue to shrink until the figures are visible again. While they are obscured, the 8s will be transformed into letters (S, P, E, U, or H), and a new letter will also appear. Your job is to search for the letter U or H — it has an equal chance of appearing where any of the 8s were, or in the new spot. Click on the picture to try it out.

Attention researchers Steven Franconeri, Andrew Hollingworth, and Daniel Simons used a similar animation to answer a key question about what attracts our attention. Recent research has led to two different hypotheses — either the appearance of a new object, or a change in the luminance (brightness) of an area of our field of view is what attracts our attention.

In many cases, both of these changes occur at the same time. For example, we’re driving down the street and a boy runs out in front of our car chasing a ball. The boy appearing in front of our car is certainly a new object, and the light reflecting off of his sweatshirt carries a different luminance value from the asphalt pavement we had been looking at.

Franconeri and his colleagues cleverly designed the animated display you just looked at to introduce a new object without a corresponding change in luminance: the object was introduced while all four objects were obscured by the blue ring, so when it appeared, there was no luminance change.

In a separate condition, the group presented the same animation, with the ring passing behind the letters, so that when the new object appeared, there was a change in luminance. They repeated the task with 2, 3, and 4 objects in the final display; in each case, participants knew that the 8s would change into letters, and that they’d be searching for a U or an H. Here are their results:

When the ring passes in front of the letters, there is no difference in reaction time, whether the target letter (U or H) appears in a new position or an old position. This suggests that the mere fact of a new letter appearing doesn’t attract our attention, because if it did, we’d react more quickly when the target letter appeared in a new position. Compare this to the control condition, where the letters are always in view:

Now, reaction time is significantly faster for the new item. So when a new item actually changes the luminance of an area in our field of view, we react faster. Franconeri and his team argue that this result supports the luminance hypothesis. They suggest that when the new item corresponds to a luminance change, we direct our attention to that new item. Then we are able to complete the task more quickly — to determine if that item is an H or a U. So a change in luminance attracts our attention, but the appearance of a new object on its own does not.

Franconeri, S.L., Hollingworth, A., & Simons, D.J. (2005). Do new objects capture attention? Psychological Science, 16(4), 275-281.

[Cognitive Daily]
5:51:56 PM    comment

Google Gift to Digital Library. Google plans to make a $3 million gift to the Library of Congress for a project that aims to digitize significant primary materials from national libraries. By KATIE HAFNER. [NYT > Technology]
5:48:45 PM    comment

Harnessing the Power of Podcast Technology to Help those with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Adultadhdpodcast.com offers free audio shows on coping with ADD/ADHD. "Abby", a high-tech worker in Seattle, has a secret. She has Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, better known by the initials ADD or ADHD. Symptoms include the inability to mainta [Online Learning Update]
5:45:37 PM    comment

Sweat is good indicator heart attack may be coming.

Sweating during physical activity or in hot weather is healthy. But when individuals begin perspiring while experiencing discomfort in their chest, arm, neck or jaw -- with little or no exertion -- it could be the onset of a heart attack, according to a new study at the University of Illinois at Chicago.

[Science Blog -]
5:26:18 PM    comment

PodCasts May Create Class Test Driving - Mike Golz, Hofstra Chronicle. If Web sites such as RateMyProfessor.com are not enough for students debating registering for certain classes, students may soon be able to preview professors by accessing PodCasts of lectures. "We could have professors do a lecture so kids could audit a [Online Learning Update]
7:58:00 AM    comment

Outsourcing to Rural America. [Slashdot] this has the potential to revitalize the hinterland in both Canada and the US and counter the megacity urbanization side effects. -- BL

7:52:09 AM    comment

Dueling simplicities. Most of my writing goes straight to the web, but my column still takes a detour through the print magazine. Usually that's no problem. I'm not a news hound. And while I'm comfortable editing myself, I enjoy the thoughtful feedback I get from Neil McAllister who, in addition to editing my column, writes his own. Every now and then, though, I wish I could have bypassed the print loop. Case in point: next week's column on two-way RSS. I wrote it last week; it will appear on InfoWorld.com tomorrow; magazine subscribers will get it after the holiday. In that column I discuss how both Microsoft and Google plan to use XML syndication for two-way data exchange and, more broadly, to bring database-like capabilities to the web of linked documents that we are all collectively building. If I'd blogged it last week, I'd look really prescient now. ... [Jon's Radio]
7:23:02 AM    comment

© Copyright 2005 Bruce Landon.
 
November 2005
Sun Mon Tue Wed Thu Fri Sat
    1 2 3 4 5
6 7 8 9 10 11 12
13 14 15 16 17 18 19
20 21 22 23 24 25 26
27 28 29 30      
Oct   Dec


Click here to visit the Radio UserLand website.

Subscribe to "Bruce Landon's Weblog for Students" in Radio UserLand.

Click to see the XML version of this web page.

Click here to send an email to the editor of this weblog.